Study finds parents who listen help children learn

A new study from Vanderbilt University says parents can help their children learn by simply listening, instead of providing all the answers.

When the kids

explain a problem themselves, the study says, they end up better understanding the solutions.

What two numbers add up to 51, where one of the numbers is five more than the other? Seven-year-old Daniel Harriss, of Nashville, Tenn., was stumped in

the middle of math homework. So, like many kids who depend on parents for homework help, he called out to his mom, Heather, for assistance.

His mother, a youth minister at the Belmont United Methodist Church in Nashville, said her first impulse was to launch into helpful-mom mode and explain

the solution. But, instead, she asked Daniel how he thought he should approach the question. And, soon enough, he talked his way to the answer: 23 and

28.

So much the better, say researchers at Vanderbilt University in Nashville, where a new study has discovered that children learn best when explaining a

problemโ€™s solution to their parents.

โ€œItโ€™s such a natural thing for moms to dive in and help,โ€ said Bethany Rittle-Johnson, a Vanderbilt professor of psychology who supervised the study, but

parents should resist the urge.

โ€œItโ€™s really effective to try to get kids to explain things themselves, instead of just telling them the answer. Explaining their reasoning, to a parent

or other people they know, will help them understand the problem and apply what they have learned to other situations.โ€

In their study, Rittle-Johnson, along with colleagues Megan Saylor and Kathryn Swygert, showed 50 4- and 5-year-olds a pattern of colored bugs: two red

caterpillars, followed by a blue spider, two more red caterpillars and, again, a blue spider. What bug comes next? they were asked.

kb_listen_03.jpg

A child puzzles over homework.
(Photo by Bart Hoffstein)

If kids answered incorrectly, they were told the answer. (It would be a red caterpillar, in case youโ€™re flummoxed.) The

children were next divided into three groups, one where the kids just repeated the correct answer, one where they orally explained the puzzle quietly to

themselves, and the last where they talked about it to their mothers, who listened without giving input.

All three groups were then given another pattern test, a more difficult one. And Rittle-Johnson found that the group that had explained the answers to the

first puzzle to parents did better on the advanced test.

โ€œWhen weโ€™re asked to articulate ideas to other people, we learn better,โ€ Rittle-Johnson said. โ€œHaving mom there gets you to think more and talk more about

the task at hand.โ€

Despite these results from the research lab, it may be hard for many parents to adopt the studyโ€™s findings at home. โ€œOn a problem when it seems so

obvious, and youโ€™re getting a little impatient, it can be hard to say nothing,โ€ Danielโ€™s mother said.

For Alexandra Sundman, a mother of two in New York City, the balance between helping and helping too much is hard to achieve when it comes to her

4-year-old-daughter, Madeline. Some questions simply require more attention, as when Madeline asked the other day, โ€œIs God everywhere?โ€

Sundman said she tried to respond as best as she could. โ€œIโ€™m not sure any mind–a 4-year-old or a 94-year-old–can really answer that question,โ€ she told

the child. But on more fact-based questions like โ€œIf water turns into ice when itโ€™s cold, what happens to ice when itโ€™s warm,โ€ Sundman said she lets Madeline

take the lead.

Still, parents notice that kids seem to respond well to the technique used in the study. When his wooden block constructions kept tumbling down, Sean Fay,

a 4-year-old living in Fairport, N.Y., turned for help to his mom, Jennifer, saying โ€œWhy is my building falling down, and why is it wobbly?โ€ Jennifer, the

mother of three, responded by asking Sean what sort of thing would make a house more stable, something like a wide foundation, maybe.

โ€œI was trying to send him some hints without sitting down and building a new building for him,โ€ she said. She noticed, too, that the lessons sunk in when

she overheard Sean explaining building strategies to his 2 1/2-year-old sister, Rachel. โ€œIf you donโ€™t want it to fall down, you have to give it a sturdy

base,โ€ he said.

Parents who resist the urge to take over may find that there are rewards for hanging back. โ€œFrom experiencing the success of it, it lets me be more

patient because I know heโ€™s going to be able to figure it out,โ€ said Harriss, the mother in Nashville. โ€œItโ€™s fun to be able to celebrate with them when they

see they did it.โ€

Rittle-Johnson said she believes this type of learning can also produce benefits in the classroom, where children who passively received instruction could

now be more actively engaged in the learning process.

And some teachers have already attested to the benefits of Rittle-Johnsonโ€™s approach. By asking students to teach one another in the classroom, Dana

Luria, a teacher at the Bronx Guild in New York City, helps learners become more involved with the lesson. โ€œIt gives students confidence in their work and

forces them to work through the steps in greater depth,โ€ Luria said.

Whatever the benefits for children, the new approach can also take pressure off parents to be walking encyclopedias. โ€œYour kids ask you questions all the

time that you donโ€™t know the answers to, and this suggests that itโ€™s OK if you donโ€™t, itโ€™s fine.โ€ Rittle-Johnson said. โ€œIf you just say, โ€˜What do you think

the answer is?โ€™ you can help kids learn.โ€

Hot this week

Evolution Dome and TDS deliver 137 pods for major healthcare conference

Inflatable breakout rooms provide flexible, soundproof meeting spaces for...

How The Daily Telegraph Became a Mouthpiece for Zionism and Lost Its Credibility

Once considered one of the most influential and respected...

Topics

Related Articles

Popular Categories

spot_imgspot_img